Family Law Psychology Briefs

"A J.M. Craig Press Publication"

Volume 5, Number 3

 

 

======================

A note from the Editor

======================

 

Welcome to "Family Law Psychology Briefs", a quarterly publication of

J.M. Craig Press, Inc. You are receiving this Brief via email as one

of our registered subscribers. For any questions regarding your

subscription or to change your email address, please contact

<mailto:support@jmcraig.com>.

 

We recommend that this Brief be viewed with a fixed pitch font. This

Brief may also be retrieved as a web page from our "Subscribers Only

Archive" area at <http://www.jmcraig.com/subscribers/archives.htm>.

 

 

============================================================

Small Amounts of Substance Use, Human Judgment and Parenting

 

Claude Schleuderer, Ph.D.

============================================================

 

Introduction

 

Drug and alcohol use is rampant in contemporary society.  More than 50

million people in this country use alcohol, and 7 million adults use

illegal substances monthly (Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services

Administration [SAMSHA], 2002).  The effect of substance use has been

researched in a variety of fields of human endeavor including the

workforce (Lennox, Steel, Zarkin & Bray, 1998), professional

impairment (Brooke, 1997), domestic violence (Dalton, 2001), child

abuse (Kelleher, Chaffin, Hollenberg & Fischer, 1994), and family

functioning (Brooks, Gaines, Mueller & Jenkins, 1998; Haugland &

Havik, 1998). Notably, its chronic abuse has had a documented affect

on parenting (Horvath, Logan & Walker, 2002).  What is not generally

recognized is that small quantities of substances of abuse can have a

significant negative impact on the judgment that underlies parenting.

 

Contrary to the assumption that substance abuse problems either exist

in an individual or they do not, the current understanding of

substance abuse is that it is not a black or white dichotomy, but

rather exists in shades of grey.  The current thinking is that

problems with substance abuse occur along a continuum (Kruger,

Nichols, Hicks, Markon, Patrick, Iacono, McGue, 2004; Stoltenberg,

Mudd, Blow & Hill, 1998, Vinson, 1997), and the negative impact on

human judgment of small amounts of substances of abuse is just

beginning to become understood.  The feelings and perceptions that are

generated within the person ingesting the substances of abuse is, of

course, their main attraction; periods of intoxication are generally

accompanied by a feeling of euphoria or energization.  Those are the

sensations that the person ingesting the substance overtly

experiences.  However, the more subtle effects of ingesting relatively

small quantities of substances of abuse on the central nervous system

and the resultant impact on human judgment are just beginning to

become understood.

 

Executive Functions

 

The neural architecture of the human brain historically has been an

area of interest, but with the availability of MRIs and PET scans,

much more data has become available in recent years.  Among the more

interesting developments in neuropsychology has been the concept and

underlining neuropsychological research support for the discrete

presence of what has become understood as Executive Functions

(Goldberg, 2001).  The basic idea is that there exists a function of

the brain whose responsibility is to coordinate and control all the

other functions.  Some conceptualize this function as being similar to

the 'CEO' of a corporation; others have found the function of a

conductor of an orchestra to be a useful explanatory metaphor.  The

CEO's or conductor's responsibility is to direct the resources of the

organization or orchestra to work together (Gioia, Isquith & Guy,

2001).  The Executive Functions represent a neuropsychological

construct that is responsible for problem solving, anticipation,

planning, monitoring, and use of feedback (Struss & Benson, 1986;

Welch & Pennington, 1988).  More recently this concept has been

further developed to include initiating and inhibiting behavior,

meaningfully solving complex problems, cognitive flexibility, and the

ability to monitor and evaluate ones own behavior (Gioia, Isquith, Guy

& Kenworthy, 2000; Luria, 1980).  It seems the descriptions of

Executive Functions are very close to what might be more generally

understood as "judgment".

 

These Executive Functions seem to be directly related to the

pre-frontal lobes of the brain and develop both neurologically and in

terms of their impact on behavior in a way that is very different from

the rest of the brain (Goldberg, 2001).  Longitudinal MRIs suggest

that structural changes occur in the pre-frontal cortex during

adolescence and that these are responsible for an individual's

increased ability to appropriately direct his own behavior.  This

would seem to be the neuropsychological basis for the poor judgment

that is usually associated with adolescence. (An excellent discussion

of this topic appears in the APA Amicus Curiae brief in Roper

v. Simmons.)

 

The pre-frontal cortex plays a significant role in maintaining

concentration, foresight, management of risk, and directing behaviors

(Weinstein & Shaffer, 1993). Studies show damage to the pre-frontal

cortex to impair the Executive Functions (Damasio & Anderson, 2003).

Traumatic brain injury to the pre-frontal lobes (Fletcher,

Ewing-Cobbs, Miner, Levin & Eisenberg, 1990), disease processes

(Malloy & Bihrle, 1993), dementia (Harvey & Dahlman, 1999), and

chronic substance abuse involving alcohol and other drugs (Morgenstern

& Bates, 1999) have been demonstrated to negatively impact these

Executive Functions.  Impaired Executive Functioning results in

impaired judgment.

 

It is important to understand that while a good deal of the Executive

Functions are related to the pre-frontal cortex, the current

understanding is that these functions are accomplished because of the

extreme connectivity of the pre-frontal cortex to all the other parts

of the brain (Gioia & Isquith, 2002).  This view underscores the

unique capacity of the pre-frontal cortex to direct and coordinate the

neural transmissions to and between other parts of the brain.  This

means that the pre-frontal cortex's strength relies on its ability to

act as a neurotransmitter switchboard between various other parts of

the brain.  It is through this connectivity with other Central Nervous

System (CNS) functions that the Executive Functions are able to

organize and direct all cognitive activity.

 

Role of Dopamine

 

There are several substances present in the brain that have roles in

transmitting electrical impulses from one part of the brain to

another.  Called neurotransmitters, these are the electro-chemical

messengers between the neurons in the CNS.  They send signals from one

neuron to another by their presence, or absence, in the spaces between

the neurons, the so-called synapses.  Dopamine is one of these

neurotransmitters upon which this CNS communication is dependent.

Current wisdom holds that behavior is controlled by two separate and

distinct systems: one system responsible for initiating behavior; and

a parallel system that is responsible for inhibiting behavior

(Patterson & Newman, 1993). Dopamine seems to be intricately involved

in the inhibitory, rather than initiating system, as evidenced by,

among other factors, dopamine's heavy implication in both Tourette's

Syndrome and ADHD (Rogeness, Javors & Pliska, 1992; Singer & Walkup,

1991), conditions that are associated with impairments in the

inhibitory system.

 

There is now considerable evidence that substances of abuse alter

dopamine production (Carlson, 1994) in ways that interfere with

neurotransmission, interfere with CNS functioning, and therefore

impact behavior.  The relationship between various substances of abuse

and dopamine is rather complex, but efficient cognitive functioning is

related to a rather narrow window of activation of dopamine in the

CNS: either too much or too little of this neurotransmitter impairs

the neurological system it is servicing (Jentsch & Taylor, 1999).

There is also evidence that dopamine activity is highly concentrated

in the prefrontal cortex, so upsetting the dopamine balance will

likely have a disproportional effect on the functions controlled by

that part of the brain (Cummings, 1995).  As explained above,

partially because the pre-frontal cortex's job is so dependent upon

neurotransmission, this change in dopamine activity likely has a

disproportional impact on the Executive Functions as opposed to other

cognitive functions (Lyvers, 2000).

 

Substances of abuse alter dopamine activity either directly or

indirectly.  Studies in laboratory animals indicate relatively low

doses of alcohol stimulate dopamine release in the CNS of laboratory

animals (Eckardt, File, Gessa, Grant, Guerri, & Hoffman, 1998).  Human

studies on the effect of relatively low doses of alcohol have found

significant effect of alcohol on the person's ability to inhibit

behavior while leaving intact her ability to initiate behavior (DeWit,

Crean & Richards, 2000; Fillmore & Vogel-Sprott, 2000).  Weissenborn

(2003) also found that relatively small amounts of alcohol effects

judgment and reduces a person's ability to inhibit undesired behavior.

These findings support the proposition that low doses of alcohol

effect dopamine and, as a result, impair the inhibition function.

Additionally, it has been found (Pihl, Paylan, Gentes-Hawn & Hoaken)

that the negative effects of alcohol on the Executive Functions become

more pronounced as blood alcohol levels decrease after a period of

even mild intoxication.  This finding is troubling because is suggests

that parenting judgment continues to be impaired even when there is no

trace of alcohol in the urine or blood.  Of these studies Fillmore

(2003) observes, "What is perhaps most remarkable about these findings

is the reliable impairing effects on inhibitory control in spite of

the relatively simple nature of the inhibitory response tested in the

model and the comparatively mild alcohol doses administered in these

studies...  The findings support the notion that inhibitory mechanisms

of behavioral control can be particularly sensitive to the impairing

effects of alcohol at doses that do not affect the ability to quickly

and accurately execute behavior." Stimulants (or uppers) seem to have

a similar effect on inhibiting responses (Evenden 1999).

 

Cocaine, either as coke or crack, also impacts the dopamine system.

It enhances dopamine production and potently blocks its reuptake into

the neuron (Basso & Bornstein, 2000; Bates & Convit, 1999).  Keeping

in mind that human efficiency occurs within a rather narrow window of

dopamine activity, consequences to the efficient operation of the

Executive Functions necessarily follow.

 

The active ingredient in marijuana and hashish is THC, which has been

shown to be an antagonist of dopamine, with this effect being most

evident in the prefrontal cortex (Jensch, et al 1999).  The

neurobehavioral effects associated with marijuana intoxication

involving cognitive flexibility and speed of information processing

(Basso & Bornstein, 2000) support the effect of this substance on the

Executive Functions.

 

Amphetamines, with street names such as speed, meth, or crystal, are

also antagonists on the dopamine system.  They cause more dopamine to

be in the synapses during intoxication but deplete the cell's supply

of this neurotransmitter long after the end of the acute intoxication

phase (Basso & Bornstein, 2000).  This finding is especially important

because it suggests that the impairment outlasts the period of

intoxication.  Also of concern is the finding that, in animal studies,

dopamine activity was atypical in animals who had been exposed to

amphetamines over a single eight hour period for as long as 3 weeks

after their exposure (Basso & Bornstein, 2000).

 

The ability to direct attention is one of the Executive Functions, and

substances of abuse affect attention (Koelega, 1993).  The purpose of

attention is to direct a person's resources toward those things that

are important and away from distractions. The research suggests that

the capacity to control attention can become impaired at relatively

low doses of stimulants in otherwise healthy individuals (Koelega,

1993).  Ecstasy (MDMA) has also recently become the focus of

experiments concerning attention.  Studies are few and far between,

but at least one study (Lamers, Ramaekers, Muntjewerff, Samyn, Read,

Brookhuis & Reidel, 2003) found that, while simple attention tasks may

have benefited from even small recreational doses of the drug, more

complex tasks requiring judgment were significantly impaired.  This

study found that judgment was impaired even with a single dose of

MDMA.

 

Impact of Substance Use on Parenting

 

Filmore (2003) notes that laboratory findings regarding the impact of

substances of abuse are somewhat misleading.  First of all, the

effects identified are produced by relatively mild doses of the abuse

drugs, doses "much lower than are commonly self administered in

recreational use." Lamers et al. (2003) essentially made the same

observation regarding other recreational drugs.  Secondly, the

laboratory studies test relatively simple and highly identifiable,

measurable discrete behaviors.  In the real world, behaviors are more

complex and occur in environments that have more distractions, and the

specific responses that a situation calls for are far less clear

cut. Consequently, when the underlying issues and behaviors are

considerably more complex, the expectation would be that the impact of

even low doses of substances of abuse would be considerably greater

than the studies have found.  It is also important to realize that the

cognitive effects of substances of abuse can outlast the period of

acute intoxication.  For example, it has been found that use of

psychoactive substances can impair cognitive functioning sometimes for

as long as three weeks after the last ingestion (Vik, Cellucci,

Jarchow & Hedt, 2004).

 

Parenting is probably one of the most challenging human activities

because, in many daily situations, parents must choose between a wide

variety of parenting behaviors, many with both short and long term

consequences. Moreover, these choices must be made in situations that

are often highly complex and emotionally charged.  Behavioral choices

must be made within a short period of time, and the demands of

parenting are unrelenting.  In other words, good parenting requires

good judgment, and good judgment requires fully operational Executive

Functions.

 

Low doses of substances of abuse impair judgment. Applying the

impaired judgment that comes from even low doses of substances of

abuse to the complex array of subtle judgments that are necessary for

good parenting yields impaired parenting.  In any parenting situation,

a parent must decide what to do and must also decide what not to do,

but poor judgments are amplified when they occur in the context of a

parent whose inhibitory capacities are also impaired due to the

substance use.  Family law professionals should keep these effects in

mind when assessing cases where there are indications of parents

uncharacteristically lashing out at their children or each other

either verbally or physically even when there are no other overt

indications of substance intoxication. Of greater concern is the

finding that these effects may occur when there are no other outward

indications of intoxication and likely last longer than the period of

acute intoxication.

 

RECOMMENDATIONS

 

The point to underscore is that even low doses of substances of abuse

have a negative effect on parenting.  Furthermore, the negative

effects outlast the period of acute intoxication and are surprisingly

long lasting.  Many courts write orders that parents who may have

substance abuse issues should not ingest substances for 24 hours prior

to having access to their children.  This may not be enough time to

clear the effects of the substances on judgment.

 

It also follows that parents who are responsible for their children

around the time that they are ingesting even small quantities of

substances of abuse are not operating at the peak efficiency and

strength of their judgment abilities.  Consequently, Family Court

professionals should carefully assess the substance use patterns of

all potential custodial resources and enter even recreational

substance use into the decision-making matrix regarding custodial

access.

 

REFERENCES

 

Basso, M. R., & Bornstein, R.A. (2000).  Neurobehavioral consequences

of substance abuse and HIV infection.  Journal of Psychopharmacology,

14, 228-237.

 

Bates, M. E., & Convit, A. (1999). Neuropsychology and neuroimaging of

alcohol and illicit drug abuse.  In A. Calev (Ed.), Assessment of

Neuropsychological Functions in Psychiatric Disorders (pp. 373-446).

Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

 

Brooke, D. (1997). Impairment in the medical and legal professions.

Journal of Psychosomatic Research, 43, 27-34.

 

Brooks, P. H., Gaines, L. S., Mueller R., & Jenkins, S. (1998).

Children's television watching and their father's drinking practices.

Addiction Research, 6, 27-34.

 

Carlson, N. R. (1994).  Physiology of behavior.  Boston: Allyn &

Bacon.

 

Cummings, J. L. (1995).  Anatomic and behavioral aspects of

frontal-subcortical circuits.  Annals of the New York Academy of

Sciences, 769, 1-13.

 

Dalton, B. (2001). Batterer characteristics and treatment completion.

Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 16, 1223-1238.

 

Damasio, A. R., & Anderson S. W. (2003). The Frontal Lobes. In

K. M. Heilman & E. Valenstein (Eds.) Clinical Neuropsychology, Fourth

Edition (pp. 404-446 ). New York: Oxford University Press.

 

DeWit, H., Crean, J., & Richards, J.B. (2000). Effects of

d-amphetamine and ethanol on a measure of behavioral inhibition in

humans. Experimental and Clinical Psychopharmacology, 4, 5-10.

 

Eckardt, M.J., File, S.E., Gessa, G.L., Grant, K.A., Guerri, C., &

Hoffman, P. L. (1998). Effects of moderate alcohol consumption on the

central nervous system.  Alcoholism, Clinical and Experimental

Research, 22, 998-1040.

 

Evenden, J. L. (1999). Varieties of impulsivity.  Psychopharmacology,

146, 348-361.

 

Fillmore, M.T. (2003). Drug abuse as a problem of impaired control:

Current approaches and findings. Behavioral and Cognitive Neuroscience

Reviews, 2, 179-197.

 

Fillmore, M. T., & Vogel-Sprott, M. (2000).  Response inhibition under

alcohol: Effects of cognitive and motivational conflict.  Journal of

Studies on Alcohol, 61, 239-246.

 

Fletcher, J. M., Ewing-Cobbs, L., Miner, M. E., Levin, H. S., &

Eisenberg, H. (1990). Behavioral changes after closed head injury in

children. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 58, 93-98.

 

Gioia, G. (2004). Understanding executive functions: definitions,

assessment & interventions.  Paper presented to Ulster County Mental

Health Psychology Department, February 17, 2004.

 

Gioia, G., & Isquith, P. (2002). New Perspective on educating children

with ADHD: Contributions of the executive functions. Journal of Health

Care Law & Policy, 5, 124-163.

 

Gioia, G., Isquith, P., & Guy, S.  ( 2001). Assessment of executive

functions in children with neuropsychological impairment.  In

R. Simeonsson & S. Rosenthal (Eds.) Psychological and Developmental

Assessment: Children with Disabilities and Chronic Conditions (pp

317-356). New York: Guilford Press.

 

Goldberg, E. (2001). The Executive Brain: Frontal Lobes and the

Civilized Mind. New York: Oxford University Press.

 

Harvey, P. D., & Dahlman, K. L. (1999). Neuropsychological assessment

in dementing conditions. In Avram Calev (Ed.) Assessment of

Neuropsychological Functions in Psychiatric Disorders (pp. 329-372).

Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press.

 

Haugland, B. S., & Havik, O. E. (1998). Correlates of family

competence with paternal alcohol abuse. Psychological Reports, 83,

867-880.

 

Horvath, L., Logan, T., & Walker, R. (2002). Child custody cases: A

content analysis of evaluations in practice. Professional Psychology:

Research and Practice, 33, 557-565.

 

Jentsch, J. D., & Taylor, J. R. (1999). Impulsivity resulting from

frontostriatal dysfunction in drug abuse: Implications for the control

of behavior by reward-related stimuli.  Psychopharmacology, 146,

373-390.

 

Kelleher, K., Chaffin, M., Hollenberg, J., & Fischer,

E. (1994). Alcohol and drug disorders among physically abusive and

neglectful parents in a community-based sample. American Journal of

Public Health, 84, 1586-1590.

 

Koelega, H. S. (1993). Stimulant drugs and vigilance performance: A

review.  Psychopharmacology, 111, 233-249.

 

Krueger, R. F., Nichol, P. E., Hicks, B. M., Markon, K. E., Patrick,

C. J., Iacono, W.G., & McGue, M. (2004). Using latent trait modeling

to conceptualize alcohol problems continuum.  Psychological

Assessment, 16,107-119.

 

Lamers, C. T., Ramaekers, J. G., Muntjewerff, N. D., Samyn, N., Read,

N. L., Brookhuis, K. A., & Reidel, W. J. (2003). Dissociable effects

of a single dose of ecstasy (MDNA) on psychomotor skills and

attentional performance.  Journal of Psychopharmacology, 17, 379-387.

 

Lennox, D. R., Steel, P. D., Zarkin, G. A., & Bray, J. W. (1998). The

differential effects of alcohol consumption and dependence on adverse

alcohol related consequences; Implications for the workforce. Drug and

Alcohol Dependence, 50, 211-220.

 

Luria, A. R. (1980) Higher Cortical Functions in Man.  New York: Basic

Books.

 

Lyvers, M. (2000).  "Loss of control" in alcoholism and drug

addiction: A neuroscientific interpretation.  Experimental and

Clinical Psychopharmacology, 8, 225-249.

 

Malloy, P., Bihrle, A., Duffy, J., & Cimino C. (1993). The

orbitomedial frontal syndrome Archives of Clinical Neuropsychology, 8,

185-201.

 

Morgenstern, J., & Bates, M. E. (1999). Effects of executive function

impairment on change processes and substance abuse outcome in 12-step

treatment. Journal of Studies on Alcohol, 60, 846-855.

 

Patterson, C. M., & Newman J. P. (1993).  Reflectivity and learning

from aversive events: Towards a psychological mechanism for the

syndrome of disinhibition.  Psychological Review, 100, 716-736.

 

Pihl, R. O., Paylan, S. S., Gentes-Hawn, A., & Hoaken,

P. N. (2003). Alcohol affects executive functioning differentially on

the ascending versus descending limb of the blood alcohol

concentration curve.  Alcoholism: Clinical and Experimental Research,

27, 773-779.

 

Rogeness, G. A., Javors, M. A., & Pliska, S. R. (1992). Neurochemistry

in child and adolescent psychiatry. Journal of the American Academy of

Child and Adolescent Psychiatry, 31, 765-781.

 

Singer, H. S., & Walkup, J. T. (1991). Tourette syndrome and other tic

disorders. Medicine, 70, 15-32.

 

Stoltenberg, S. F., Mudd, S. A., Blow, F. C., & Hill,

E. M. (1998). Evaluating measures of family history of alcoholism:

Density versus dichotomy. Addiction , 93, 1511-1520.

 

Struss, D. T., & Benson, D. F. (1986). The Frontal Lobes. New York:

Raven.

 

Substance Abuse and Mental Health Services Administration {SAMHSA}.

(2002). Results from the 2001 National Household Survey on Drug Abuse:

Summary of National Findings (Office of Applied Studies, NHSDA Series

H-17, DHHS Publication NO SMA 02-3758). Rockville, MD: Author.

 

Vik, P. W., Cellucci, T., Jarchow, A., & Hedt, J. (2004). Cognitive

impairment in substance abuse. Psychiatric Clinics of North America,

27, 97-109.

 

Vinson, D. C. (1997). Alcohol is not a dichotomous variable. Journal

of Family Practice, 44, 147-149.

 

Weinstein, C. S., & Shaffer, H. J.  (1993).  Neurocognitive aspects of

substance abuse treatment.  Psychotherapy: Theory, Research, Practice

and Training, 30, 317-333.

 

Weissenborn, R. D. (2003).  Acute alcohol effects on cognitive

functioning in social drinkers: Their relationship to drinking

habits. Psychopharmacology, 165, 306-312.

 

Welsh, M. C., & Pennington, B. F. (1988) Assessing frontal lobe

functioning in children: Views from developmental

psychology. Developmental Neuropsychology, 4, 131-

 

 

===================

Copyright Statement

===================

 

Entire contents Copyright 2004, J.M. Craig Press, Inc. All Rights

Reserved.

 

For additional information regarding this and our other publications,

please visit our website at <http://www.jmcraig.com/>.

 

 

 

 

Return to Table of Contents

 

Last updated January 26, 2011

 

Links to other sites, or links to this site from any other sites, do not imply any endorsement of, or relationship with, such other sites.